Informacja

Drogi użytkowniku, aplikacja do prawidłowego działania wymaga obsługi JavaScript. Proszę włącz obsługę JavaScript w Twojej przeglądarce.

Tytuł pozycji:

Investigating illegal activities that affect biodiversity: the case of wildlife consumption in the Brazilian Amazon.

Tytuł:
Investigating illegal activities that affect biodiversity: the case of wildlife consumption in the Brazilian Amazon.
Autorzy:
Chaves WA; Princeton School of Public and International Affairs, Princeton University, Robertson Hall, Princeton, New Jersey, 08544, USA.; Department of Fish and Wildlife Conservation, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, 310 West Campus Drive, Blacksburg, Virginia, 24061, USA.; Núcleo de Estudos e Pesquisas das Cidades da Amazônia Brasileira, Universidade Federal do Amazonas, Avenida Rodrigo Otávio, 6200, Coroado, Campus Universitário/Setor Norte/ICHL/NEPECAB, Manaus, AM, 69080-900, Brazil.
Valle D; School of Forest, Fisheries and Geomatics Sciences, University of Florida, McCarty Hall C, Gainesville, Florida, 32011, USA.
Tavares AS; Núcleo de Estudos e Pesquisas das Cidades da Amazônia Brasileira, Universidade Federal do Amazonas, Avenida Rodrigo Otávio, 6200, Coroado, Campus Universitário/Setor Norte/ICHL/NEPECAB, Manaus, AM, 69080-900, Brazil.
von Mühlen EM; Departamento de Ecologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Avenida Senador Salgado Filho, 3000, Natal, RN, 59078-970, Brazil.
Wilcove DS; Princeton School of Public and International Affairs, Princeton University, Robertson Hall, Princeton, New Jersey, 08544, USA.; Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, 08544, USA.
Źródło:
Ecological applications : a publication of the Ecological Society of America [Ecol Appl] 2021 Oct; Vol. 31 (7), pp. e02402. Date of Electronic Publication: 2021 Aug 11.
Typ publikacji:
Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Język:
English
Imprint Name(s):
Publication: Washington, D.C. : Ecological Society of America
Original Publication: Tempe, AZ : The Society, 1991-
MeSH Terms:
Animals, Wild*
Conservation of Natural Resources*
Animals ; Biodiversity ; Brazil ; Cities ; Humans
References:
Bernard, H. R. 2011. Research methods in anthropology: qualitative and quantitative approaches. Fifth edition. AltaMira Press, Lanham, Maryland, USA.
Blair, G., K. Imai, and J. Lyall. 2014. Comparing and combining list and endorsement experiments: Evidence from Afghanistan. American Journal of Political Science 58:1043-1063.
Blair, G., K. Imai, and Y.-Y. Zhou. 2015. Design and analysis of the randomized response technique. Journal of the American Statistical Association 110:1304-1319.
Bockenholt, U., S. Barlas, and P. G. M. van der Heijden. 2009. Do randomized-response designs eliminate response biases? An empirical study of non-compliance behavior. Journal of Applied Econometrics 24:377-392.
Brancalion, P. H. S., D. R. A. de Almeida, E. Vidal, P. G. Molin, V. E. Sontag, S. E. X. F. Souza, and M. D. Schulze. 2018. Fake legal logging in the Brazilian Amazon. Science Advances 4:eaat1192.
Brasil. 1967. Lei no. 5197, de 3 de janeiro de 1967. Dispõe sobre a proteção à fauna e dá outras providências. Diário Oficial da União, Brasília, Brasil, 5 jan. 1967.
Brasil. 1998. Lei no. 9605, de 12 de fevereiro de 1998. Dispõe sobre as sanções penais e administrativas derivadas de condutas e atividades lesivas ao meio ambiente, e dá outras providências. Diário Oficial da União, Brasília, Brasil 13 fev. 1998.
Carvalho, E. A. R. 2019. Jaguar hunting in Amazonian extractive reserves: acceptance and prevalence. Environmental Conservation 46:334-339.
Cerri, J., A. Ciappelli, A. Lenuzza, A. Nocita, and M. Zaccaroni. 2017. The randomised response technique: A valuable approach to monitor pathways of aquatic biological invasions. Fisheries Management and Ecology 24:504-511.
Chang, C. H., M. Cruyff, and X. L. Giam. 2018. Examining conservation compliance with randomized response technique analyses. Conservation Biology 32:1448-1456.
Chang, C. H., S. J. Williams, M. X. Zhang, S. A. Levin, D. S. Wilcove, and R. C. Quan. 2019. Perceived entertainment and recreational value motivate illegal hunting in Southwest China. Biological Conservation 234:100-106.
Chaves, W., D. Valle, and D. Wilcove. 2021. Dataset, R codes, and Models in Jags to accompany paper Investigating illegal activities that affect biodiversity: the case of wildlife consumption in the Brazilian Amazon. University Libraries, Virginia Tech, data set. https://doi.org/10.7294/qe42-td57.
Chaves, W., D. Wilcove, A. Tavares, D. Valle, and T. Morcatty. 2020. Survey tools for research conducted in the Amazon. University Libraries, Virginia Tech, data set. https://doi.org/10.7294/jk6j-2q18.
Chaves, W. A., D. S. Wilkie, M. C. Monroe, and K. E. Sieving. 2017. Market access and wild meat consumption in the central Amazon, Brazil. Biological Conservation 212:240-248.
Cialdini, R. B. 2007. Descriptive social norms as underappreciated sources of social control. Psychometrika 72:263-268.
Davis, E. O., B. Crudge, T. Lim, D. O'Connor, V. Roth, M. Hunt, and J. A. Glikman. 2019. Understanding the prevalence of bear part consumption in Cambodia: A comparison of specialised questioning techniques. PLoS ONE 14:e0211544.
Di Minin, E., et al. 2019. Identifying global centers of unsustainable commercial harvesting of species. Science Advances 5:eaau2879.
Droitcour, J., R. A. Caspar, M. L. Hubbard, T. L. Parsley, W. Visscher, and T. M. Ezzati. 1991. The item count technique as a method of indirect questioning: A review of its development and a case study application. Pages 185-210 in P. P. Biemer, R. M. Groves, L. E. Lyberg, N. A. Mathiowetz, and S. Sudman, editors. Measurement errors in surveys. John Wiley & Sons, New York, New York, USA.
El Bizri, H. R., et al. 2020. Urban wild meat consumption and trade in central Amazonia. Conservation Biology 34:438-448.
Fowler, F. 2002. Survey research methods. Third edition. Sage, Thousand Oaks, California, USA.
Fox, J. P., and R. R. Meijer. 2008. Using item response theory to obtain individual information from randomized response data: an application using cheating data. Applied Psychological Measurement 32:595-610.
Fox, J. A., and P. E. Tracy. 1986. Randomized response. Sage, Beverly Hills, California, USA.
Gingerich, D. W., V. Oliveros, A. Corbacho, and M. Ruiz-Vega. 2016. When to protect? Using the crosswise model to integrate protected and direct responses in surveys of sensitive behavior. Political Analysis 24:132-156.
Gjestvang, C. R., and S. Singh. 2007. Forced quantitative randomized response model: a new device. Metrika 66:243-257.
Godoy, R., E. A. Undurraga, D. Wilkie, V. Reyes-Garcia, T. Huanca, W. R. Leonard, T. McDade, S. Tanner, V. Vadez, and T. B. S. Team. 2010. The effect of wealth and real income on wildlife consumption among native Amazonians in Bolivia: estimates of annual trends with longitudinal household data (2002-2006). Animal Conservation 13:265-274.
Greenberg, B. G., R. R. Kuebler, J. R. Abernathy, and D. G. Horvitz. 1971. Application of randomized response technique in obtaining quantitative data. Journal of the American Statistical Association 66:243-250.
Groenitz, H. 2014. A new privacy-protecting survey design for multichotomous sensitive variables. Metrika 77:211-224.
Gupta, S., B. Gupta, and S. Singh. 2002. Estimation of sensitive level of personal interview survey questions. Journal of Statistical Planning and Inference 100:239-247.
Higgs, S. 2015. Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite 86:38-44.
Hinsley, A., A. Keane, F. A. V. St John, H. Ibbett, and A. Nuno. 2019. Asking sensitive questions using the unmatched count technique: Applications and guidelines for conservation. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 10:308-319.
Holbrook, A. L., and J. A. Krosnick. 2010. Measuring voter turnout by using the randomized response technique evidence calling into question the method's validity. Public Opinion Quarterly 74:328-343.
Huang, K. C., C. H. Lan, and M. P. Kuo. 2005. Detecting untruthful answering in randomized response sampling. Quality & Quantity 39:659-669.
IBGE. 2010. Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Cidades@. Brasília, Brasil. http://www.cidades.ibge.gov.br/xtras/home.php.
IBGE. 2016. Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Cidades@. Brasília, Brasil. https://cidades.ibge.gov.br.
IBGE. 2017. Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Sistema IBGE de Recuperação Automática. Brasília, Brasil. https://sidra.ibge.gov.br/Tabela/3175.
Johnson, T. P., and F. J. R. van de Vijver. 2003. Social desirability in cross-cultural research. Pages 195-204 in J. A. Harkness, F. J. R. van de Vijver, and P. P. Mohler, editors. Cross-cultural survey methods. Wiley, New York, New York, USA.
Krumpal, I. 2009. Sensitive questions and measurement error: using the randomized response technique to reduce social desirability bias in CATI surveys. Universitat Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany.
Krumpal, I. 2012. Estimating the prevalence of xenophobia and anti-Semitism in Germany: A comparison of randomized response and direct questioning. Social Science Research 41:1387-1403.
Nuno, A., and F. John. 2015. How to ask sensitive questions in conservation: A review of specialized questioning techniques. Biological Conservation 189:5-15.
Parry, L., J. Barlow, and H. Pereira. 2014. Wildlife harvest and consumption in Amazonia's urbanized wilderness. Conservation Letters 7:565-574.
Plummer, M., A. Stukalov, and M. Denwood. 2016. Package ‘rjags': bayesian graphical models using MCMC. http://mcmc-jags.sourceforge.net/.
Pollock, K. H., and Y. Bek. 1976. Comparison of 3 randomized response models for quantitative data. Journal of the American Statistical Association 71:884-886.
PPI. 2019. Poverty Probability Index. https://www.povertyindex.org/.
R Core Team. 2020. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. www.R-project.org.
Razafimanahaka, J. H., R. K. B. Jenkins, D. Andriafidison, F. Randrianandrianina, V. Rakotomboavonjy, A. Keane, and J. P. G. Jones. 2012. Novel approach for quantifying illegal bushmeat consumption reveals high consumption of protected species in Madagascar. Oryx 46:584-592.
Scheffers, B. R., B. F. Oliveira, I. Lamb, and D. P. Edwards. 2019. Global wildlife trade across the tree of life. Science 366:71-76.
Schreiner, M. 2010. A Poverty Probability Index (PPI®) for Brazil (2008). http://www.povertyindex.org/country/brazil.
Schwarz, N. 2003. Self-reports in consumer research: The challenge of comparing cohorts and cultures. Journal of Consumer Research 29:588-594.
Solomon, J., S. K. Jacobson, K. D. Wald, and M. Gavin. 2007. Estimating illegal resource use at a Ugandan park with the randomized response technique. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 12:75-88.
Stets, J. E., and P. J. Burke. 2000. Identity theory and social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly 63:224-237.
Susan, G. W. L., W. F. Laurance, E. M. N. Henrique, A. Andrade, P. M. Fearnside, R. G. R. Expedito, and R. Condit. 2009. Long-term variation in Amazon forest dynamics. Journal of Vegetation Science 20:323-333.
Tian, G. L., J. W. Yu, M. L. Tang, and Z. Geng. 2007. A new non-randomized model for analysing sensitive questions with binary outcomes. Statistics in Medicine 26:4238-4252.
Tourangeau, R., L. J. Rips, and K. Rasinski. 2000. The psychology of survey response. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Tourangeau, R., and T. Yan. 2007. Sensitive questions in surveys. Psychological Bulletin 133:859-883.
Warner, S. L. 1965. Randomized-response - a survey technique for eliminating evasive answer bias. Journal of the American Statistical Association 60:63-69.
Wilkie, D. S., and R. A. Godoy. 2001. Income and price elasticities of bushmeat demand in lowland Amerindian societies. Conservation Biology 15:761-769.
Wu, C. H., S. M. Wan, and M. C. Li. 2008. Randomized response model in a matched pair study. Journal of Statistical Planning and Inference 138:2059-2066.
Yu, J. W., G. L. Tian, and M. L. Tang. 2008. Two new models for survey sampling with sensitive characteristic: design and analysis. Metrika 67:251-263.
Contributed Indexing:
Keywords: illegal behavior; randomized response technique; risk perception; social desirability bias; unrelated question design; wildlife consumption; wildlife trade
Entry Date(s):
Date Created: 20210707 Date Completed: 20211020 Latest Revision: 20211020
Update Code:
20240105
DOI:
10.1002/eap.2402
PMID:
34233059
Czasopismo naukowe
The illegal use of natural resources, manifested in activities like illegal logging, poaching, and illegal wildlife trade, poses a global threat to biodiversity. Addressing them will require an understanding of the magnitude of and factors influencing these activities. However, assessing such behaviors is challenging because of their illegal nature, making participants less willing to admit engaging in them. We compared how indirect (randomized response technique) and direct questioning techniques performed when assessing non-sensitive (fish consumption, used as negative control) and sensitive (illegal consumption of wild animals) behaviors across an urban gradient (small towns, large towns, and the large city of Manaus) in the Brazilian Amazon. We conducted 1,366 surveys of randomly selected households to assess the magnitude of consumption of meat from wild animals (i.e., wild meat) and its socioeconomic drivers, which included years the head of household lived in urban areas, age of the head of household, household size, presence of children, and poverty. The indirect method revealed higher rates of wildlife consumption in larger towns than did the direct method. Results for small towns were similar between the two methods. The indirect method also revealed socioeconomic factors influencing wild meat consumption that were not detected with direct methods. For instance, the indirect method showed that wild meat consumption increased with age of the head of household, and decreased with poverty and years the head of household lived in urban areas. Simultaneously, when responding to direct questioning, households with characteristics associated with higher wild meat consumption, as estimated from indirect questioning, tended to underreport consumption to a larger degree than households with lower wild meat consumption. Results for fish consumption, used as negative control, were similar for both methods. Our findings suggest that people edit their answers to varying degrees when responding to direct questioning, potentially biasing conclusions, and indirect methods can improve researchers' ability to identify patterns of illegal activities when the sensitivity of such activities varies across spatial (e.g., urban gradient) or social (e.g., as a function of age) contexts. This work is broadly applicable to other geographical regions and disciplines that deal with sensitive human behaviors.
(© 2021 by the Ecological Society of America.)

Ta witryna wykorzystuje pliki cookies do przechowywania informacji na Twoim komputerze. Pliki cookies stosujemy w celu świadczenia usług na najwyższym poziomie, w tym w sposób dostosowany do indywidualnych potrzeb. Korzystanie z witryny bez zmiany ustawień dotyczących cookies oznacza, że będą one zamieszczane w Twoim komputerze. W każdym momencie możesz dokonać zmiany ustawień dotyczących cookies